Domhoff, G.W. (1996). Finding meaning in dreams. A quantitative approach. r.1 The scientific study of dream content.pdf

(144 KB) Pobierz
Finding Meaning in Dreams: Chapter 1
Strona 1 z 6
Use these links to quickly navigate through the book:
¤ Introduction
¤ Chapter 1: The Scientific Study of Dream Content
¤ Chapter 2: The Hall/Van De Castle System
¤ Chapter 3: The Quality of the Data
¤ Chapter 4: Normative Findings
¤ Chapter 5: Age Differences in Dream Reports
¤ Chapter 6: Cross-Cultural Studies of Dream Content
¤ Chapter 7: Consistency and Change in Long Dream Series
¤ Chapter 8: The Continuity Between Dreams and Waking Life
¤ Chapter 9: The Repetition Dimension
¤ References
Chapter 1: The Scientific Study of Dream Content
Introduction
The purpose of this book is to search for meaning in dreams through the quantitative study of
dream content. It will do so in three senses of that elusive term. First, it will demonstrate an internal
coherence or regularity in the dreams of specific groups, such as men, children, or members of
hunting and gathering societies. Second, it will show there is consistency in what individuals dream
about from year to year and even over decades. Third, it will reveal correspondences between dream
content and waking life; more specifically, it will show a direct continuity between dream concerns
and waking concerns.
The book will attempt to realize its purpose through the version of content analysis created by
Calvin S. Hall and Robert L. Van de Castle (1966) on the basis of earlier work by Hall (1947, 1951,
1953a) and his students (e.g., Polster, 1951; Reis, 1951; Meer, 1955; Cook, 1956; Paolino, 1964).
The Hall/Van de Castle coding system was constructed gradually through the empirical study of
thousands of dream reports collected from college students in the 1940s and 1950s. It is the most
comprehensive and detailed system for the study of dream content developed to date. Given the
explicit coding rules developed by Hall and Van de Castle, all of which will be presented in this
book, their categories can be used by any investigator willing to take the time to learn the system.
The Hall/Van de Castle system may be unique among methods of dream analysis in that it relies
entirely on the dream reports themselves in order to determine whether or not there is meaning in
dreams. It does not use free associations, amplifications, biographical information, or any other
information provided by the dreamer. Nor does it draw upon metaphoric, linguistic, or literary
methods of interpretation. The Hall/Van de Castle system makes comparisons of dream reports in
three different ways to search for dream meaning. First, it compares new dream reports with
normative information on American college students and other population groups. Second, it
compares one dream or type of dream within an individual dream series with other dreams in the
series. Third, it compares reports of specific types of dreams collected from many people, such as
dreams of flying or of appearing partially clad in public, with each other and with the norms for
dreams in general.
The quantitative study of dream content begins with the careful formulation of categories to
encompass the many different elements appearing in dreams. In the case of the Hall/Van de Castle
file://E:\prace\artykuþy\sny\ksiazki\finding_meaning_in_dreams\Finding Meaning in ...
2009-02-11
998108692.018.png 998108692.019.png 998108692.020.png 998108692.021.png 998108692.001.png 998108692.002.png 998108692.003.png 998108692.004.png 998108692.005.png 998108692.006.png 998108692.007.png 998108692.008.png 998108692.009.png
 
Finding Meaning in Dreams: Chapter 1
Strona 2 z 6
system, these categories include characters, social interactions, settings, the activities engaged in
by the dreamer and other dream characters, and a wide range of objects. There are also coding
categories for emotions, temporal references, successes and failures, good fortunes and misfortunes,
and many other aspects of dream content. Categories can be expanded or combined to fit the needs
of specific research questions, and new categories can be created.
As will be shown, the Hall/Van de Castle system has a number of advantages over other coding
systems, and in fact encompasses most other systems. For one thing, it is possible to achieve high
intercoder reliability. For another, its categories have been shown to be psychologi-cally relevant in
terms of the waking concerns of those who have contributed dream reports. Furthermore, its
normative findings on the dream reports of American college students, replicated several times,
provide a comparison point for studies of dream content all over the world. It therefore has been used
by investigators in many different countries, including India and Japan, for a wide range of projects.
This widespread use makes it possible for findings to be cumulative and therefore to serve as a
reference point for new studies.
Studies of over 10,000 dream reports using the Hall/Van de Castle system have yielded consistent
developmental, gender, and cross-cultural differences as well as a core of findings stable across
gender lines and cultural boundaries. Studies of lengthy dream diaries from a diverse array of
individuals reveal that there are large individual differences in dream content as well as a high
degree of consistency in what a person dreams about over the space of several months or years, or
even 40 and 50 years in the cases of the two longest dream series analyzed to date. There are also
striking continuities between dream content and waking life, making possible accurate predictions
about the concerns and interests of the dreamers.
In order to link dream content with the waking thoughts and behavior of the dreamer, the Hall/Van
de Castle system makes one basic assumption: the frequency with which a dream element appears
reveals the concerns and interests of the dreamer. That is, frequency is assumed to be an indicator of
intensity (Hall and Van de Castle, 1966:13--14). The idea that frequency reveals concerns and
interests means any statistically significant deviation from the norms in either a high or low direction
should relate to psychologically unique aspects of the dreamer's waking thoughts or behavior. The
formula for the significance of differences between two independent proportions is used to determine
statistically significant deviations from the norms; more importantly, the magnitude of any
statistically significant differences, that is, the "effect size," is determined by Jacob Cohen's (1977:
chap. 6) "h" statistic. For readers unfamiliar with statistics, these methods are explained in an
appendix. To make it unnecessary for readers to calculate either of these two statistics, the appendix
includes tables from which they can be determined with considerable ease. This statistical appendix
is best read in conjunction with the introduction to chapter 4.
Dreams and Dream Reports
Just what is being studied when we say we are analyzing "dream" content? The word "dream" has
three possible meanings. It can refer to (1) an experience during sleep; (2) what is remembered upon
awakening; or (3) what is reported to others, usually prefaced by "I had this dream" or "last night I
dreamt that..." Put another way, there is an experienced dream, a remembered dream, and a reported
dream. Unfortunately, there is no way of knowing what people are dreaming or what they remember
until they report their recall of the dreaming experience in words. Thus, only the reported dream has
an objective, or public, existence. Of necessity, then, this book will deal only with the reported
dream, usually referred to as a "dream report" or "dream narrative." The phrase "dream content" will
be used to designate what is found in the dream report (Hall and Van de Castle, 1966: 17-18; Hall
and Nordby, 1972: 12).
Dream reports are a unique type of document. Most of what is spoken or written is meant to
file://E:\prace\artykuþy\sny\ksiazki\finding_meaning_in_dreams\Finding Meaning in ...
2009-02-11
998108692.010.png 998108692.011.png
Finding Meaning in Dreams: Chapter 1
Strona 3 z 6
influence other people or communicate with them, but dream reports are descriptive accounts
based on memories of an experience that happened during sleep. On occasion they might be used to
influence a psycho-therapist or communicate something to a friend, but for the most part they are
"representational," not "instrumental," communications. Moreover, dream reports of the kind used by
dream researchers are not generally self-initiated. That is, very few of the people who provide dream
reports for our studies would have written down their dreams if they had not been asked to do so,
although those who keep dream diaries for their own personal reasons are an important exception.
Then too, because dreams are usually experienced as something that just happens to the dreamer, and
not as something intended, people do not tend to accept as much responsibility for their dreams as
they do for what they say or write during waking life. In all these ways, dream reports differ from
other types of oral or written reports (Hall and Van de Castle, 1966: 21).
The quantitative study of dream content is not without its difficulties. There can be problems of
bias in the collection of dream reports; for example, aspects of dream content may be altered or
deemphasized on the basis of instructions given to the dreamer. Nor is there any guarantee that some
subjects do not make up dream reports or alter details of what they actually remember. However,
none of these problems is a serious one. Most of the dream reports utilized in the studies to be
discussed in this book were collected from anonymous volunteer subjects with basically the same
instructions. As for the fabrication of some dreams in a sample, or the deliberate alteration of certain
details, this presents no problem in terms of our general findings because they are based on very
large numbers of dream reports.
Dream Series and Dream Sets
Dream reports can be grouped in two different ways, as "dream series" or as "dream sets." A
"dream series" consists of two or more dream reports from the same person; a "short" dream series
contains anywhere from two to 99 dream reports, and a "long" dream series contains 100 or more
dream reports. The phrase "dream set" is used to describe both (1) dream reports from persons of a
certain "type" (e.g., men, women, children, schizophrenics, Americans) or (2) dream reports of a
certain "type" collected from a wide range of people (e.g., dreams of being chased, dreams of
falling). The same dream reports are sometimes part of both a series and a set. For example, if we
took all dream reports of being chased from hundreds of different dream series, we would have a set
of chase dreams. For our purposes, the most important example of a dream set drawn from many
dream series concerns the normative studies of the dream reports of European-American college men
and women reported in detail in chapter 4. This normative study is based upon five dream reports
drawn at random from longer series contributed by 100 men and 100 women at Case Western
Reserve University and Baldwin-Wallace College in Cleveland, Ohio, between 1947 and 1950 (Hall
and Van de Castle, 1966:158).
The Importance of "Blind Analysis"
It is our strong belief that content analyses should be done with no knowledge of the dreamer if
such studies are to be convincing evidence for the usefulness of content analysis. Such "blind
analyses" are essential because there is always the possibility, even with this objective and
quantitative method, that the analyst is reading into the dream reports what she or he already knows
from free associations or biographical information, rather than gaining new insights and information
from the dream reports themselves.
It is of course necessary and inevitable that a dream analyst will have other information if he or
she is treating the dreamer in a clinical setting. But such clinical analyses, however beneficial
therapeutically or useful in generating testable hypotheses about dream meaning, always will be
suspect by rigorous scientific standards. This is not only due to the fact the analyst has other
file://E:\prace\artykuþy\sny\ksiazki\finding_meaning_in_dreams\Finding Meaning in ...
2009-02-11
998108692.012.png 998108692.013.png
Finding Meaning in Dreams: Chapter 1
Strona 4 z 6
information available that might be influencing interpretations, but because such dream analyses
are a form of "post hoc" interpretation based on reasoning from the present to the past. The criticism
of "retrospective" analyses as unable to demonstrate the existence of causality is a cross borne by all
clinical theories.
Unfortunately, it is not possible to answer this type of criticism with experimental studies in the
case of dreams. Experimental studies of the cognitive process of dreaming have shown that some
external (e.g., water drops, hearing significant names) and internal (e.g., thirst) stimuli can
sometimes have a modifying influence on dream content, and heightened emotional or motivational
states can change the general vividness or emotionality of dreams (e.g., Berger, 1963; Witkin and
Lewis, 1967; Witkin, 1969; Bokert, 1967; Hoelscher, et al., 1981). However, beyond a general
demonstration of the psychological lawfulness of the dream process, experimental studies have not
been able to tell us very much about the meaning of most dream content or about the relationship
between dream content and either waking thought or behavior (cf. Antrobus, 1990: 4, and
Cartwright, 1990:179).
Given the limited usefulness of retrospective analyses on the one hand and experimental studies
on the other in the study of dream content, blind analyses using quantitative methods become the
best approach to the scientific study of dream meaning. As we will see, this approach is especially
compelling when dream series are used to make many specific predictions about a person's
conceptions, concerns, and interests. In those cases where the dreamer is known to the researcher,
predictions should be made in advance about the nature of the dream content and the dream reports
should be coded by someone who does not know the predictions.
Not all the studies reported in this book are based on blind analyses, but in many studies the
person doing the quantitative content analysis knew only the gender and/or nationality of the
dreamer, or was unaware of the purpose of the study. Whatever the limitations of previous studies,
however, the future clearly lies with blind analyses of dream content if the study of dream meaning
is to be taken seriously by social scientists.
Dreaming and Daydreaming
Many different questions can be asked about dreaming and dreams, and there is a large literature
on each of them. Since the discovery in 1953 of two different types of sleep--active, fast-wave
Rapid-Eye-Movement (REM) sleep and slow-wave, quiescent Non-REM (NREM) sleep--there have
been thousands of psychophysiological studies of sleep and dreams by researchers all over the world
(e.g., Aserinsky and Kleitman, 1953; Dement, 1955; Dement and Kleitman, 1957; Hartmann, 1967;
see Ellman and Antrobus, 1991, for one major summary). In psychological terms, these researchers
are searching for physiological correlates of dreaming. Some of them believe dreaming occurs
almost exclusively in REM sleep, and that we therefore know something about the neurophysiology
of dreaming (e.g., Hobson, 1988; Hobson and McCarley, 1977; McCarley, 1989), but others dispute
this conclusion, claiming that the presence of fully developed dreams shortly after sleep onset and
during NREM sleep suggests we still know little about the psychophysiology of the dream process
(e.g., Foulkes, 1962, 1985, 1993a; Vogel, 1978; Cavallero, et al., 1992; Cicogna, 1994).
There is also an important literature on dreaming as a cognitive process. It builds on the
burgeoning research in the area of waking cognition to show that much of what was previously
inexplicable about dreaming can be understood in terms of recent findings and concepts developed in
the study of waking consciousness and memory. David Foulkes has been the major contributor to
this effort; his Dreaming: A Cognitive-Psychological Analysis (1985) synthesizes what is known
about dreaming from laboratory studies with findings on waking cognition and presents many new
hypotheses for future investigation. The work of John Antrobus (1977, 1990), Harry Hunt (1986,
1989), and Donald Kuiken (1986) also is important in this area. Still others have done ingenious
file://E:\prace\artykuþy\sny\ksiazki\finding_meaning_in_dreams\Finding Meaning in ...
2009-02-11
998108692.014.png 998108692.015.png
Finding Meaning in Dreams: Chapter 1
Strona 5 z 6
experimental studies in the sleep laboratory suggesting possible psychological functions for
dreams. The major contributor to this area of study has been Harry Fiss (1983, 1986, 1991).
As fascinating and informative as this work is, it will not be discussed in this book because it does
not provide detailed answers to questions about dream content and its correspondence to waking
thought and behavior. The biology of dreaming does not tell us the psychological meaning of dreams
(cf. Fiss, 1979, 1991). The dreaming brain and the dreaming mind are two different issues, one
neurophysiological, one psychological. Similarly, the study of dreaming as a cognitive process has
made a great contribution by showing that dreams are psychologically meaningful in a general sense,
but few of these studies help us to understand the meaning of specific dream reports or the
relationship between dream content and waking thought or behavior. Then too, dreams could have
no "functions" at all in terms of either evolutionary survival or individual adaptation, but still be
psychologically meaningful in terms of internal coherence and correlations with waking thought and
behavior (e.g. Antrobus, 1993; Foulkes, 1993a).
There is also an interesting literature on daydreaming, reveries, and extraneous thoughts, based in
a variety of techniques, including thought-sampling by means of pagers carried by people going
about their everyday lives. Just as our studies show that dream content has continuities with waking
life, these studies suggest the dreamlike nature of some waking thought (e.g., Singer, 1966, 1975,
1988; Klinger, 1971, 1990, Starker, 1978; Foulkes, 1994). This literature will figure importantly in
our theoretical comments in the final chapter. Relevant findings also will be referred to at a few
places in the intervening chapters. To underscore the relationship between dream content and relaxed
waking thought, we have adopted Klinger's (1971) phrase "current concerns" because it characterizes
the major content of both forms of cognition. Our former phrase, "emotional preoccupations," will be
used interchangeably. "Unfinished business" and "unfinished intentions" also express part of this
concept, but "current concerns" and "emotional preoccupations" allow for the positive interests
sometimes appearing in dreams.
For the most part, though, this book will be focused on the quantitative study of dream content and
its relationship to such factors as gender, age, nationality, and individual differences. Its findings
stand on their own, whatever theory turns out to be right concerning the neurophysiological
correlates of dreaming, or whatever cognitive theory eventually explains the production of the
dreams reported to investigators. Similarly, there are enough differences between dreams and
daydreams to repay a primary concern on dreams as a unique experience in our lives.
The Meaning of Content Analysis
In the most general sense, "content analysis" is the search for meaningful regularities and patterns
in written documents. In principle it can be done "qualitatively," as when we use our intuition or our
general understanding of language, or it can be done "quantitatively." Historically, however, content
analysis as the phrase is used by social scientists has meant the attempt to convert verbal, written, or
other symbolic materials into numbers so statistical analysis can be performed. This purpose is
accomplished by formulating categories, tabulating frequencies for those categories, and determining
percentages, proportions, or ratios. Comparisons are then made with "norms" or control groups.
In practice, there is not a hard and fast line between the qualitative and the quantitative. One can
shade into the other. We often begin with an implicit set of categories and develop a rough idea of
the frequency of elements fitting into those categories. Next we create more explicit, carefully
defined categories, and then we make a more detailed search of the document for exact frequencies.
This is in fact the process used by Hall (1951) in his early work.
Thus, quantitative content analysis often develops out of impressionistic qualitative analyses as an
attempt to minimize personal bias and make possible greater agreement among investigators who are
file://E:\prace\artykuþy\sny\ksiazki\finding_meaning_in_dreams\Finding Meaning in ...
2009-02-11
998108692.016.png 998108692.017.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin