The truth is out there.pdf

(87 KB) Pobierz
544105886 UNPDF
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
Viren Swami and Rebecca Coles look at belief in conspiracy theories.
Given the widespread appeal of conspiracy theories, it is surprising that little empirical research has
addressed the reasons why some people are more likely to adhere to such beliefs than others. This article
reviews the available literature on the sociological and psychological antecedents of conspiracy theories, and
concludes by examining the positive and negative impacts of conspiracy theories. For scholars, studying
conspiracy theories may prove useful in better understanding the needs of individuals that conspiracy
theories accommodate, while also allowing for a better conceptualisation of the everyday effects of such
theories.
‘The truth’, the TV show The X-Files told us, ‘is out there’. Millions of people worldwide seem to agree,
disbelieving official accounts of important social and political events. In the United States, for example,
scholars have noted a steady increase in the number of poll respondents who believe that Lee Harvey
Oswald did not act alone in killing John F. Kennedy (Goertzel, 1994; McHoskey, 1995). In the wake of 9/11,
commentators highlighted the proliferation of conspiracy theories about the event (e.g. Goldberg, 2004), with
polls suggesting that more than a quarter of respondents believe the US government knew in advance
(Zogby International, 2004), participated in, or took no action to stop the attacks (Hargrove & Stempel, 2006).
But conspiracy theories are not a uniquely American phenomenon: in a poll of seven predominantly Muslim
countries, Gentzkow and Shapiro (2004) reported that almost four fifths of respondents did not believe the
9/11 attacks were carried out by Arabs, believing instead that it was the work of the US or Israeli
governments (for other conspiracy theories in the Middle East, see Zonis & Joseph, 1994). In Britain, the
BBC’s documentary series The Conspiracy Files has examined a range of theories in current circulation,
including those about the deaths of Princess Diana and UN weapons inspector David Kelly, the bombing of
PanAm Flight 103, and the London bombings of 7 July 2005.
Given such widespread belief in conspiracy theories across the globe, it comes as something of a
surprise to learn that there remains a dearth of empirical research on the topic (Abalakina-Paap et al., 1999).
Part of the problem may be that academics have traditionally not engaged with conspiracy theories for fear of
being branded as conspiracy theorists themselves. Until recently, it was not uncommon to find accounts of
possible or real conspiracies prefaced by disclaimers of the kind that Ramsay (1990) notes: ‘In intellectually
respectable company, it is necessary to preface any reference to... conspiracies with the disclaimer that the
speaker “doesn’t believe in the conspiracy theory of history (or politics)”.’
A related problem is in distinguishing between a conspiracy theory and an awareness of genuine political
conspiracies. A broad definition of the former was provided in Hofstadter’s (1966) seminal essay, ‘The
paranoid style in American politics’, where a conspiracy theory was described as a belief in the existence of a
‘vast, insidious, preternaturally effective international conspiratorial network designed to perpetrate acts of
the most fiendish character’ (p.14; for extended discussions, see Bale, 2007; Sunstein & Vermeule, 2009).
That such beliefs are relatively widespread suggests that they fulfil certain social functions or psychological
needs; given this role, conspiracy theories are deserving of the same academic study as other religious,
political or social beliefs (Bale, 2007).
The remainder of this article discusses both early work in sociology and cultural studies on the causes of
conspiracy theorising, and emerging psychological research focused on the individual difference antecedents
of conspiracy theories. We conclude with reasons why further research on conspiracy theories is important,
both in terms of academic research and sociopolitical practice.
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
544105886.001.png
Early sociological work
Hofstadter’s (1966) essay on the ‘paranoid style’, in which he examined right-wing conspiracy theories,
effectively set the tone of much of the research that was to follow. The paranoid style, Hofstadter (1971,
pp.2–3) argued, was a result of ‘uncommonly angry minds’, whose judgement was somehow ‘distorted’.
Following this vein, some scholars came to view conspiracy theories as a product of psychopathology, such
as extreme paranoia, delusional ideation or narcissism (e.g. Groh, 1987; Plomin & Post, 1997). In this view,
the incorrectness of conspiracy theories was usually assumed a priori and, more than this, the delusional
aspect of conspiratorial beliefs was thought to result in an incapacity for social or political action (e.g.
Hofstadter, 1971).
While it is possible that some people who believe in conspiracy theories suffer forms of psychopathology,
this in itself is an incomplete explanation given how widespread conspiracy theories are (Sunstein &
Vermeule, 2009; Waters, 1997). Hofstadter, however, has remained influential for his interest in why people
acquire conspiracy theories, suggesting that a belief in conspiracy theories was more likely to emerge
among those who felt powerless, disadvantaged or voiceless, especially in the face of catastrophe. To use a
contemporary example, believing that the 7/7 London bombings were perpetrated by the British or Israeli
governments may be, for some individuals at least, a means of making sense of turbulent social or political
phenomena.
To the extent that conspiracy theories fill a need for certainty, it is thought they may gain more
widespread acceptance in instances when establishment or mainstream explanations contain erroneous
information, discrepancies, or ambiguities (Miller, 2002). A conspiracy theory, in this sense, helps explain
those ambiguities and ‘provides a convenient alternative to living with uncertainty’ (Zarefsky, 1984, p.72). Or
as Young and colleagues (1990, p.104) have put it, ‘[T]he human desire for explanations of all natural
phenomena – a drive that spurs inquiry on many levels – aids the conspiracist in the quest for public
acceptance.’
In addition, it is also thought that conspiracy theories offer explanations of the world that are not
contradicted by information available to adherents. In the context of extremism, Hardin (2002) has discussed
what he calls a ‘crippled epistemology’: in some cases, extremism is not an irrational response, but rather
stems from the fact that people have very little correct or accurate information. Sunstein and Vermuele
(2009) apply a similar perspective to conspiracy theories: those who believe in conspiracy theories may be
responding rationally and logically to what little information they receive, even if that information appears
absurd in relation to wider, publicly available knowledge.
Other scholars have extended or revised Hofstadter’s original powerlessness conjecture in order to
explain how adherents come to hold conspiracy theories. Some have suggested that an inability to attain
goals leads to conspiracy theories (Edelman, 1985; Inglehart, 1987), while others view conspiracy theories
as affording adherents a means of maintaining self-esteem (e.g. Robins & Post, 1997), coping with
persecution (Combs et al., 2002), reasserting individualism (Davis, 1969; Melley, 2000), expressing negative
feelings (Ungerleider & Wellisch, 1979) or reaffirming imagined positions of exclusive knowledge (Mason,
2002). These contrasting theories, however, share the distinguishing assumption that conspiracy theories are
a rational attempt to understand complex phenomena and deal with feelings of powerlessness. In this sense,
such beliefs reveal not psychopathological minds but the lived experience and consciousness of groups of
individuals (Sanders & West, 2003).
Psychological accounts
Early psychological studies often sought to highlight characteristics of conspiracy theories themselves,
rather than characteristics of the audience. So, for example, conspiracy theories were described as being
characterised by poor or unproven evidence, circular reasoning, repetition of unproved premises, and the
creation of false predicaments (e.g. Young et al., 1990; Zarefsky, 1984). On the other hand, once the notion
that conspiracy theories serve some psychological need became established, a small number of studies
began to explicitly examine the socio-cognitive basis of those beliefs.
For example, one early study examined the effects of exposure to Oliver Stone’s 1991 film JFK, in which
it is alleged that the assassination of John F. Kennedy was a conspiracy at the highest levels of government.
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
 
The authors found that the film changed beliefs toward accepting the broad conspiracy theory and
‘significantly aroused anger’, which was explained as a function of helplessness (Butler et al., 1995, p.237).
Moreover, viewing the film was found to be associated with a decrease in viewers’ (self-reported) intention to
vote or make political contributions, suggesting that the message of the film carried over to general political
judgements.
Other research activities have focused on the psychological factors and processes associated with belief
in conspiracy theories. For example, some early work suggested that conspiracy theories emerged because
of ‘an irrational need to explain big and important events with proportionately big and important causes’
McCauley & Jacques, 1979, p.637; see also Leman, 2007). Clarke (2002), on the other hand, has discussed
conspiracy theories in the context of the fundamental attribution bias: because of the general tendency to
overestimate the importance of dispositional factors and underestimate situational factors, conspiracy
theorists are more likely to blame Hofstadter’s (1966) ‘preternaturally effective international conspiratorial
network’ even when adequate situational explanations are available. This may be especially true when
people are outraged or distressed and seek to justify their emotional state by claiming intentionality of actions
even in the absence of evidence (cf. Festinger, 1957).
Sunstein and Vermeule (2009) have suggested that the emotional content of many conspiracy theories
plays an important role in their dissemination and acceptance. They cite studies showing that ‘urban legends’
that are devised to trigger strong emotions are more likely to be spread among populations (e.g. Heath et al.,
2001). Applying this to conspiracy theories, they postulate that conspiracy theories create intense emotions
that help spread similar beliefs, while also providing a justification for affective states produced by some
traumatic event.
Other relevant work has examined the psychological impact of exposure to conspiracy theories,
particularly in relation to mass media sources (e.g. Butler et al., 1995), but also in relation to the third- person
effect (the tendency for people to believe that persuasive media has a larger influence on others than
themselves). In one study, Douglas and Sutton (2008) had participants read material containing conspiracy
theories about Princess Diana’s death before rating their own and others’ agreement with the statements, as
well as their perceived retrospective attitudes. They found that participants significantly underestimated how
much the conspiracy theories influenced their own attitudes.
In an earlier study, McHoskey (1995) predicted that conspiracy theories concerning the assassination of
JFK, and possibly all conspiracy theories, would continue endlessly because of the processes of biased
assimilation and attitude polarisation. In the first instance, when opposing sides were presented with the
same evidence, McHoskey (1995) showed that there was a tendency to uncritically accept evidence that was
supportive of one’s own argument, while scrutinising and discrediting contrary evidence. When participants
were presented with mixed evidence, there were signs of attitude polarisation, with participants reporting that
they were more in favour of their initial viewpoint, rather than reporting a reversal of their beliefs. In a similar
vein, Leman and Cinnirella (2007) found that conspiracy believers judged fictitious accounts of an
assassination more plausible if it was consistent with their beliefs, a tendency called ‘confirmation bias’.
Conspiracy believers found that ambiguous information fitted better with a conspiracist explanation, whereas
non- believers believed it suited a non- conspiracist account. In other words, the same piece of information
can be used to support very different accounts, depending on who it is presented to.
Individual differences
Perhaps one of the most important conclusions to emerge from the handful of studies to focus explicitly on
the individual antecedents of belief in conspiracy theories was Goertzel’s (1994) assertion that conspiracy
beliefs form part of a ‘monological belief system’. This allows conspiracy theorists to easily assimilate
explanations for new phenomena that would otherwise be difficult to understand or would threaten their
existing beliefs. Recent work supports this, showing that those who more strongly endorsed 9/11 conspiracy
theories were also more likely to believe in other, seemingly unrelated conspiracy theories (Swami et al., in
press).
Related work in this area has provided some support for early sociological work on conspiracy theories.
For example, studies have variously reported significant associations between conspiracist ideation and
anomie, distrust in authority, political cynicism, powerlessness and self-esteem (Abalakina-Paap et al., 1999;
Goertzel, 1994; Swami et al., in press). Interestingly, at least two studies have also reported significant
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
 
associations between conspiracist beliefs and authoritarianism (Abalakina-Paap et al., 1999; McHoskey,
1995), which the former study explained as a manifestation of the tendency of believers in conspiracy
theories to blame outgroups for problems experienced by the ingroup.
Most recently, Swami and colleagues (in press) found that 9/11 conspiracist beliefs were significantly
associated with the Big Five personality factor of Openness to Experience, with the authors suggesting that
intellectual curiosity, an active imagination, and a proclivity for new ideas results in greater exposure and
subsequent assimilation of conspiracist beliefs. Interestingly, Swami et al. (in press) also found that
individuals who more strongly believed in conspiracy theories were more supportive of democratic principles.
They went on to argue that, for participants who reject the political system as undemocratic, mainstream
explanations of social events are unsatisfactory precisely because they are provided by the very sources that
these participants doubt.
Good or bad?
What practical impact do conspiracy theories have? Some scholars (e.g. Clarke, 2002) argue that conspiracy
theories are ultimately beneficial because they reveal actual anomalies in mainstream explanations and
demand greater transparency from governments (see also Leman, 2007). The fact that some conspiracy
theories (such as US Department of Defence plans to stimulate acts of terrorism and blame them on Cuba)
have turned out to be true certainly bears out this point. Miller (2002) likewise contends that conspiracy
theories provide individuals with a public opportunity, otherwise likely denied to them, of addressing the
credibility of governments or other socio-political actors. As Fenster (1999, p.109) writes, conspiracies ‘must
be recognised as a cultural practice that attempts tomap, in narrative form, the trajectories and effects of
power’. In this view, conspiracy theories may be regarded as the beginnings of social movements that could
create positive change and foster solidarity (Sasson, 1995).
The same authors, however, are also quick to caution that conspiracy theories remain limited because
their critique of power structures is often highly simplistic. In many cases, conspiracy theories succumb to
racist or exclusionary narratives, thus losing any positive thrust. Moreover, conspiracy theories typically
threaten to unravel and ‘leave unsettled the resolution to the question of power that [they] attempt to address’
(Fenster, 1999, p.109). For Fenster (1999) and Miller (2002), in particular, conspiracy theories have the
potential to create constructive socio-political change, but also the ability to sow discord, violence and public
mistrust, while diverting attention from political issues of real significance and undermining democratic
debate.
Some scholars have also noted the negative practical effects of conspiracy theories on a range of
behaviours. Consider, for example, the conspiracy theories held by some that birth control and HIV/AIDS are
plots against African Americans (e.g. Bird & Bogart, 2003). Certainly, the history of segregation in the US, the
conducting of unethical research with African Americans (such as the Tuskegee syphilis study), and
contemporary experiences of racism help explain the existence of such theories. However, adherence to
such conspiracy theories has also been associated with less consistent pregnancy prevention and condom
use, possibly impacting upon knowledge about HIV/AIDS and AIDS prevention programmes (e.g. Bogart &
Thorburn, 2006).
Conclusion
Documenting the prevalence of conspiracy theories only provides a starting point for tackling their negative
effects. Because of their nature, beliefs in conspiracy theories have proven very difficult to repudiate (Keeley,
1999): group members may segregate themselves (informationally, though also, sometimes, physically) and
over time become increasingly distrustful of the motives of others. Kramer (1994) called this an example of a
‘sinister attribution error’: because in extreme cases they feel under constant scrutiny, individuals may
overestimate personalistic motives among others and see purposeful plots where there are in fact benign
actions.
In such a scenario, what should be the response of scholars and other interested parties? Some authors
have recently suggested possible practical means of tackling false and harmful conspiracy theories, such as
enlisting independent groups to rebut theories or ‘cognitively infiltrate’ conspiracist groups (see Sunstein &
Vermeule, 2009). The assumption here is that beliefs in conspiracy theories reflect insufficiently critical
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
 
assimilation of knowledge and that practical steps can be taken, albeit with difficulty, to counter that crippled
epistemology. On another level, however, many contemporary conspiracy theories also reflect a deep
cynicism toward, and diminished faith in, governance (Goldberg, 2004). For example, the finding that almost
a quarter of British Muslims believe that the four men blamed for the London bombings did not carry out the
attacks (Soni, 2007) reflects, in part at least, the alienation of many British Muslims from mainstream politics
and governance. A first and important step in tackling potentially harmful conspiracy theories would be to
address such causes of popular discontent.
For scholars, there remain several neglected individual difference variables, including just-world beliefs,
locus of control, subjective happiness, and possibly even paranormal beliefs. It may also prove useful to
distinguish between beliefs that reflect ‘political paranoia’ in the traditional sense, and political realism. In
doing so, it will be important for scholars to drop the assumption that all conspiracy theories are equally
unbelievable. Only by evaluating and understanding ‘both the context of the explanation and the effects of
the explanation’ (Waters, 1997, p.123) will we appreciate to what extent conspiracy theories reflect everyday
cognitions.
THE TRUTH IS OUT THERE
 
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin