22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology.pdf

(269 KB) Pobierz
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:43 PM
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology
JOHN J. OHALA
Subject
Linguistics » Historical Linguistics
Key-Topics
phonetics
DOI:
10.1111/b.9781405127479.2004.00024.x
Two of the most successful enterprises in linguistics over the past couple of centuries have been (i) in
historical linguistics, reconstruction of the prehistory of languages via the comparative method, and (ii) in
phonetics, the development of methods and theories for understanding the workings of speech, that is, how
it is produced, its acoustic structure, and how it is perceived. My purpose in this chapter is to demonstrate
that the comparative method can be refined and elaborated still more if it is integrated with modern
scientific phonetics. By incorporating phonetics it is possible to implement a research program that
genuinely constitutes “experimental historical phonology” (Ohala 1974).
1 Background
1.1 Taxonomic versus scientific phonetics
In speaking of the integration of phonetics into historical phonology it must be understood that “phonetics”
refers to what I call “scientific phonetics,” 1 not “taxonomic phonetics.” The latter is the traditional, almost
exclusively articulatory phonetics which provides linguistics with the terminology and conceptual framework
for describing speech sounds and their natural classes. This descriptive system reached a high level of
refinement in the late nineteenth century through the efforts of phoneticians such as Alexander Melville Bell,
Otto Jespersen, Paul Passy, Henry Sweet, and Wilhelm Viëtor, and its basic structure has not changed very
much since. Scientific phonetics, on the other hand, has a very long tradition, dating at least from the time
of Galen, the second-century ad anatomist, with important contributions to the present time from other
anatomists, as well as physiologists, physicists, voice teachers, engineers, linguists, and others. It constantly
accumulates new data, methods, and theories on how speech works. Moreover, it tests these theories and
continually refines the evidence adduced in support of them. When the evidence fails to support proposed
theories, it abandons them, as is true of any mature discipline. It is scientific phonetics, not taxonomic
phonetics, that needs to be better joined with historical phonology.
There have, in fact, been many prior attempts to bring about this union. Prior to instrumental studies of
speech there were some conceptions of the workings of speech which were based on impressionistic
auditory or kinesthetic sensations and on direct visual inspection. Even at this stage of development in the
nineteenth century there were some applications of phonetics to historical phonology (Bindseil 1838; Rapp
1836; von Raumer 1863; Weymouth 1856). Some of the early attempts to synthesize speech (von Kempelen
1791; Willis 1830) inspired a few works attempting to explain sound change by reference to physical
properties of speech sounds, as they were understood at the time (Jacobi 1843; Key 1855).
Instrumental study of speech on live, intact, speakers blossomed in the 1860s and 1870s. 2 It is noteworthy
that one of the motivations for such research was at its onset the attempt to understand the mechanisms of
sound change. In 1876 Rosapelly declared optimistically (I translate) that:
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g978140512747924
Page 1 of 13
665559173.007.png 665559173.008.png
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:43 PM
From the point of view of linguists, these (physiological) studies seem to be of great
importance, since their science, whose precision grows from day to day, tends to take
experimental study as its point of departure. The comparative study of different languages and
the study of the successive transformations undergone by each of them in the course of its
development have, in fact, permitted the secure formulation of certain laws that one can call
physiological and which have presided over the evolution of language.
Within a couple of decades this program produced, among other works, Rousselot's 1891 dissertation, which
was an attempt to present the physiological basis of some of the sound changes that transformed late Latin
into the regional dialect spoken in his home town.
There is still much of value to be gleaned from such early instrumental phonetic studies. One example is E.
A. Meyer's (1896–7) early discovery of the perturbations of F0 on vowels following voiced and voiceless
consonants -one of the topics that still preoccupies phoneticians both for its value to an understanding of
speech production (Löfqvist et al. 1989) and for its relevance to the phonological development of distinctive
tone from the influence of consonants (Hombert et al. 1979).
In instrumental phonetics, the discovery of the magnitude and range of lawful variation in speech must rank
as one of the major findings of linguistic science, although its full significance for an understanding of
sound change seems not yet to be fully appreciated. Having said this, it must be admitted that much of this
early work in laboratory phonetics had obvious limitations: due to technological constraints it focused almost
exclusively on the articulatory aspect of speech and neglected the acoustic and perceptual aspects. As many
modern studies have shown and as will be emphasized in this chapter, a proper understanding of sound
change requires reference to these other domains. Perhaps the one aspect of early phonetically informed
studies of sound change from which we may still draw inspiration is the expressed belief that sound change
and phonological universals may profitably be studied in the laboratory.
1.2 Constraints of the discussion
The following discussion of the mechanisms of sound change will be constrained in two ways. First, I will for
the most part be concerned only with those sound changes that are independently manifested in similar
form in different languages. The practical effect of this is to filter out changes due to language-specific or
culture-specific factors, for example, the influence of writing, regularization of morphological paradigms,
borrowing, etc. What remains is the vast majority of sound changes that have occupied phonologists’
attention over the past two centuries and which one can assume are caused by the only factors that are
common to all languages at all periods of time: the physical phonetic properties of the speech production
and perception systems. Second, I will focus primarily on the initiation of sound change, that is, the factors
that lead to variant pronunciation norms in the first place, not the subsequent spread or transmission of a
novel norm through the speech community or through the lexicon. The factors influencing the spread of a
sound change are social and psychological and may very well involve language and culture-specific factors.
(However, see Ohala 1995c for speculations on phonetic factors influencing some aspects of the spread of a
sound change.)
2 The Phonetic Basis of Sound Change
2.1 Sound change and synchronic phonetic variation
Detailed phonetic studies present us with two fundamental facts that force us to try to understand sound
change by looking carefully at the phonetics of speech production and speech perception. The first of these
is that there is a huge amount of variation in the way the “same” phonological unit is pronounced, whether
this unit is the phone, syllable, or word. The relatively short list of allophones given in conventional
phonemic descriptions of languages is just the “tip of the iceberg.” 3 Fine-grained instrumental analyses of
speech, especially recent acoustic studies, reveal that the variation is essentially infinite, though generally
showing lawful dependency with respect to the phonetic environment, speech-style, or characteristics of the
individual speaker (Lindblom 1963; Moon and Lindblom 1994; Sproat and Fujimura 1993; Sussman et al.
1991). Most of this variation is difficult to notice perceptually except through the use of controlled listening
tests (e.g., Ohala and Feder 1994). Even after a great deal of ingenious quantitative analysis (Bladon et al.
1984; Miller 1989; Peterson 1951; Syrdal and Gopal 1986) there does not yet exist a universally applicable
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g978140512747924
Page 2 of 13
665559173.009.png
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:43 PM
1984; Miller 1989; Peterson 1951; Syrdal and Gopal 1986) there does not yet exist a universally applicable
way to normalize this variation in vowels, that is, to extract the linguistically relevant “sames” posited for the
speech signal. Until such normalizations are understood, the validity of most posited phonological units
remains in doubt. It is this situation that I was referring to when I stated above that the wealth of phonetic
variation discovered by instrumental phonetics confronts linguistics with a problem that it has yet to deal
with.
The second fundamental fact that motivates us to look at phonetics for an understanding of sound change is
that a great deal of phonetic variation parallels sound change, that is, synchronic variation, including that
which we find in present-day speech, resembles diachronic variation. The synchronic variation can be found
both in speech production and in speech perception.
2.2 Variation in speech production
For example, the fundamental frequency of vowels is perturbed by the voicing of preceding consonants:
higher initial F0 being found after voiceless consonants and lower initial F0 after voiced ones (Meyer 1896–
7). This parallels the conditioning of new tones in a number of languages (Edkins 1864; Maspero 1912;
Hombert et al. 1979). Svantesson (1983) provides examples of this from two related dialects of Kammu, one
of which has preserved the voicing of initial stops and the other of which has lost the voicing but has
acquired a tonal distinction; see (1):
(1)
Southern Kammu Northern Kammu Translation
klaaŋ
kláaŋ
‘eagle’
glaaŋ
klàaŋ
'stone’
A second example is the fact that the intensity and the duration of the noise element in the release burst of
[t] is greater preceding the high close vowel [i] or the glide [j] than it is before other vowels (Olive et al.
1993: 286; Ohala 1989). This finds a parallel in the phonological histories of numerous languages, for
example, Tai (Li 1977) and Bantu (Guthrie 1967–71) as well as English, where stops develop affricated
releases before high, close vowels, as exemplified in (2):
(2)
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g978140512747924
Page 3 of 13
665559173.010.png 665559173.001.png
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:43 PM
Table 22.1 Data from Ikalanga showing that distinctive aspiration has developed on stops that
appeared before the Proto-Bantu super-close vowels but not before the next lower vowels
A third example is the finding that voice onset following a voiceless stop release is longer before high, close
vowels than before low, open vowels (Halle and Smith 1952; Klatt 1975; Ohala 1981b). A diachronic parallel
to this is the development of distinctive aspiration on voiceless stops in Ikalanga (Mathangwane 1996). In
Ikalanga (and many other Bantu languages) distinctive aspiration on certain stops arose out of the height
neutralization of the quality of the two highest front and back vowels, as shown in table 22.1 .
2.3 Variation in speech perception
Listeners occasionally make errors in perceiving speech. This is especially true when there is minimal higher-
level redundancy from pragmatics, semantics, syntax, and the lexicon. Such a situation is easily duplicated in
laboratory-based confusion studies where isolated nonsense syllables are presented to listeners for
identification. The results from one condition of one published study by Winitz et al. (1972) is given in table
22.2 .
The confusions shown in table 22.2 parallel some common, well-documented sound changes, as given in
table 22.3 . The parallels include the pairs of sounds involved, the phonetic environment (especially, whether
the stops are found in palatal or labial environments - where the palatalization or labialization is provided by
secondary articulations or by adjacent vowels or glides), and, in some cases, even the asymmetry in the
direction of the change (/p j / > /t/ is attested but not */t/ > /p j /). 4 Many other examples could be given
(see Ohala 1981a, 1993a, 1995b).
To recapitulate:
i much variation can be found in speech production and speech perception;
ii much of this variation parallels sound change.
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g978140512747924
Page 4 of 13
665559173.002.png 665559173.003.png
22. Phonetics and Historical Phonology : The Handbook of Historical Linguistics : Blackwell Reference Online
12/11/2007 03:43 PM
Table 22.2 Probabilities of identification of initial consonants as /p/, /t/, /k/ in the columns of
the stimuli in the rows
Table 22.3 Examples of sound changes involving large changes in place of articulation
But these two facts immediately raise the question: could this synchronic variation actually be sound change
observed “on the hoof”? Logically this would be difficult to accept, because if this were the case then we
would find sound change progressing at a rate very much faster than we do - in fact, several orders of
magnitude faster than present evidence suggests. All of the sound changes that transformed Proto-Indo-
European over five or six millennia into the present-day Indo-European languages would be accomplished in
a day or less. Somehow pronunciation remains relatively stable over time in spite of the great variation seen
in everyday speech. But if present-day variation is not sound change, then how do we account for the
uncanny similarities between them?
2.4 Variation in speech production = sound change?
The beginnings of a resolution of this paradox comes from experimental phonetics, specifically from studies
of speech perception. Several studies have shown that listeners’ judgments about what it is that they hear in
the speech signal are influenced by the context in which the sounds occur. Pickett and Decker (1960)
showed that listeners’ differentiation between topic and top pick is influenced by the rate at which the
sentence containing these utterances is spoken. Ladefoged and Broadbent (1957) showed that listeners
would identify the same vowel stimulus as /I/ or /ε/ depending on the F1 values of vowels in a precursor
sentence.
http://www.blackwellreference.com/subscriber/uid=532/tocnode?id=g9781405127479_chunk_g978140512747924
Page 5 of 13
665559173.004.png 665559173.005.png 665559173.006.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin